Communication about genetically modified foods and models of attitude change
The importance of people’s perceptions regarding information sources has been
shown through an experiment using the Elaboration Likelihood model described
earlier (Frewer
et al., 1999). This research built on the observation that, in the
UK, government sources have been shown to be one of the least trusted
providers of information about food-related risk, and consumer organisations
one of the most trusted sources, (Frewer
et al., 1996).
It is interesting to manipulate information so that the true effects of these
differences in source characteristics in terms of their influence on attitudes can
be empirically examined. A part of this process is to ensure that information is
attributed to a source that might realistically produce it in the ‘real world’. In the
experimental work reported here, both the government and consumer
organizations have produced information about genetic modification, adding
to the ‘ecological validity’ of experiment. Perceptions of risk relevance were
also manipulated as part of the experiment. This was either high (respondents
were told that they were able to buy genetically modified food in shops at the
time of the experiment) or low (respondents were told that genetically modified
foods would not be available for many years). The persuasive strength of the
information was also manipulated to be high or low. In this experiment,
persuasive information was directed towards public acceptance of genetically
modified foods. To this end, the experimental work was conducted in two stages.
The first of these was the pre-selection of messages of high and low persuasive
strength directed towards acceptance of genetic modification in food and
agriculture, using a separate group of participants. This enabled identification of
the ten most, and ten least, persuasive statements. The second was the systematic
examination of the interaction of perceived risk relevance, persuasive strength,
and trust in information source to which the information was attributed. People’s
attitudes towards genetically modified foods were then assessed. A three-factor
experimental design was used. Levels of the first factor (persuasive content)
were either high or low. Levels of the second factor (source) meant that
respondents received information, which was attributed to either a consumer
organisation or to government. The third factor (risk relevance) also had two
levels – respondents were provided with information that led them to believe
that the products of genetic modification were immediately available, or were
likely to be available only at a later date, currently far in the future. All
respondents then rated the information for their perceptions of source
characteristics and informational qualities. Assessments were also taken of
their attitudes to genetic engineering used in food production (Frewer
et al.,
1997; Bredahl
et al., 1998), as well as completing other tasks designed to assess
the extent to which they had internalised the information.
It was found that the information was more trusted if it was both high in
persuasive strength and attributed to the government, if irrelevant risks were
presented to respondents. However, it was found that highly persuasive
information from a consumer organisation, or information from government
that was low in persuasive strength, was more trusted if the risks were presented
as being highly relevant to respondents. However, in terms of differences in
attitude towards genetic modification, there were few significant differences
between conditions. Thus it seemed that information had an impact on trust or
distrust, rather than trust influencing attitude change following presentation of
information. This might be because people had very strong attitudes towards
genetic modification, and their reactions to the information (and the information
sources) were influenced by these views. Indeed, social judgement theory would
predict that initial attitude is likely to be one of the most important determinants
of reactions to persuasive information, particularly if it appears to be promoting
what might be interpreted as a vested interest (Eagly and Chaiken, 1993).
In a further experiment, (Frewer, Scholderer, and Bredahl, in press), two
kinds of information about genetically modified food were presented to
participants in an intervention trial – these were ‘product specific’ information
(which was used to present genetically modified foods in a positive light) and
‘balanced’ information, (which discussed the potential risks and benefits of
genetic modification of foods in a very neutral, but probabilistic and technical
way). The information was attributed either to a consumer organisation
(shown to be highly trusted in pilot research), an industry association (highly
distrusted), or the European Commission (moderately trusted) in the different
experimental conditions used in the study. Attitudes towards genetically
modified foods were assessed before and after the information intervention.
Data about people’s perceptions of information source characteristics (for
example, whether they were trusted or not) were also collected. The results
indicated that the extent to which people trusted information sources had little
impact on attitudes towards genetically modified products or product
acceptance. Prior attitudes towards genetically modified foods accounted
for almost 95 and 90 per cent of the variance in perceived benefit and
perceived risk respectively. Contrary to what might have been predicted, trust
had negligible impact on these risk-related attitudes. The extent to which
participants trusted the information sources was predominantly determined by
already existing attitudes held by participants towards genetically modified
foods. Attitudes were not influenced by perceptions of source characteristics.
In other words, independent of the type of information provided, information
provision in itself had little effect on people’s attitudes towards genetically
modified foods. Perceptions regarding information source characteristics did
not contribute to attitude change, nor did the type of information strategy
adopted have an impact on post-intervention attitudes.
Of greatest concern to industry and other institutions with an interest in
information dissemination was the finding that the extent to which people
trusted the information sources was driven by people’s attitudes to genetically
modified foods. Trust did not influence the way that people reacted to the
information. On the other hand, attitudes were used to define people’s
perceptions regarding the motivation of the source providing the information.
This perhaps is understandable in the case of the product-specific information,
which was very positive about genetic modification, focusing only on benefits
associated with novel products. People who favour the use of genetic
modification are more likely to trust a source promoting its benefits. On the
other hand, people who do not support the use of genetic modification in food
production are more likely to distrust this same source providing the positive
information because it does not align with their strongly held views. This does
not explain why the same effect was observed in the case of the ‘balanced’
information strategy. The reason may be because of the way in which the
information strategies were developed in the first place from the opinions of
experts in the area of biotechnology, who proposed a ‘rationalistic’ approach to
technology communication issues. Expert views regarding what is salient to risk
communication may be very different from what is considered important by the
public.